Skip directly to site content Skip directly to page options Skip directly to A-Z link Skip directly to A-Z link Skip directly to A-Z link
Volume 23, Number 8—August 2017
Research Letter

Effects of Influenza Strain Label on Worry and Behavioral Intentions

Author affiliations: University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa, USA (A.M. Scherer); University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, USA (M. Knaus, B.J. Zikmund-Fisher); Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, the Netherlands (E. Das); University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah, USA (A. Fagerlin)

Cite This Article

Abstract

Persons who read information about a hypothetical influenza strain with scientific (H11N3 influenza) or exotic-sounding (Yarraman flu) name reported higher worry and vaccination intentions than did those who read about strains named after an animal reservoir (horse flu). These findings suggest that terms used for influenza in public communications can influence reactions.

Influenza strains are referred to in several ways by infectious disease experts, public health officials, clinicians, and the media when communicating with the public. These influenza strain labels can focus on where the strain originated (e.g., Spanish flu); the animal reservoir (e.g., avian/bird flu), or the hemagglutinin and neuraminidase surface proteins of the strain (e.g., H1N1 influenza).

Changes in terms used to describe a health risk can shape responses to those risks (15). For example, using metaphors to describe influenza (e.g., the flu as an army invading the body) may increase influenza vaccination intentions of the public compared to literal descriptions (e.g., the flu is a virus infecting the body) (1). Labels could affect health behavior by the emotional responses they evoke (e.g., worry about infection) as a result of the terms used (6).

We tested how influenza labels affect vaccination intentions and worry about infection in a number of countries that have different cultures (1), vaccination policies (2), and experience with epidemics (3,7). After receiving exempt status from the University of Michigan Medical School institutional review board, we randomly recruited adults from a panel of internet users identified by using Survey Sampling International (SSI) (https://www.surveysampling.com/). Users were from 11 countries, the United States (n = 1,787) and 10 countries in different regions of Europe: northern [Finland (n = 1,554), Sweden (n = 1,539), Norway (n = 764)]; southern [Italy (n = 1,509), Spain (n = 1,604)]; eastern [Hungary (n = 998), Poland (n = 1,509)]; and western [Germany (n = 1,546), the Netherlands (n = 1,938), the United Kingdom (n = 1,762)]. We established quotas for age and gender to approximate the distribution of these characteristics in each country. Participants received modest compensation.

Respondents read a mock news article, ostensibly from an interview with a national health organization of the participant’s country, describing the spread of a pandemic influenza strain within their country (Technical Appendix). Each article contained information about the spread, symptoms, and severity of the virus and about the development of a vaccine.

Each version of the article referred to the influenza strain by using 1 of 3 randomized labels: 1) “H11N3 influenza,” a surface protein label; 2) “horse flu,” an animal reservoir label; or 3) “Yarraman flu,” an exotic-sounding label (Yarraman is an Australian aboriginal term for “horse”). We used novel labels to avoid associations with and reactions to established influenza labels. The study included additional factors that were cross-randomized with the label factor and are not discussed here.

After reading the article, participants were asked to imagine that the described scenario was actually occurring and then rate the level of their worry about contracting influenza and plans to receive vaccination once a vaccine for this strain of influenza became available. Responses were on 7-point scales; higher values indicated greater worry or vaccination intentions. We tested for main effects of reactions to labels by using 1-way measured analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni-adjusted planned contrasts. We used additional 2-way ANOVA tests to determine whether effects of the label manipulation differed across countries. We used the PROCESS macro for IBM SPSS Statistics 23 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) to conduct a mediation analysis and test for the effect of labels on vaccination intentions, controlling for worry.

Of 20,138 participants, 16,510 (82.0%) completed the full survey. The average participant age was 46.8 (range 18–99, SD 16.2) years; 49.8% were female.

Figure

Thumbnail of Regression coefficients for the effect of influenza labels on worry for infection and intentions for vaccination. Label conditions were dummy coded to estimate the effects of “H11N3 influenza” (X1) and “Yarraman flu” (X2) labels compared with the “horse flu” label. The effect of influenza labels on vaccination intentions, controlling for worry, is in parentheses. **p<0.01; ***p<0.001.

Figure. Regression coefficients for the effect of influenza labels on worry for infection and intentions for vaccination. Label conditions were dummy coded to estimate the effects of “H11N3 influenza” (X1) and “Yarraman...

Participants reported higher levels of worry about contracting the influenza strain when it was reported as “Yarraman flu” (mean 3.86, SD 1.83) or “H11N3 influenza” (mean 3.83, SD 1.82) compared with “horse flu” (mean 3.74, SD 1.86; F statistic [2–16,339] = 7.73, p<0.001). Participants also reported higher vaccination intentions when the strain was reported as “Yarraman flu” (mean 4.67, 1.99) or “H11N3 influenza” (mean 4.66, SD 2.03) compared with “horse flu” (mean 4.54, SD 2.04, F[10–16,339] = 6.48; p = 0.002). The effect of the influenza label on vaccination intentions was mediated by worry (Figure). Despite differences in reports of worry (F[10–16,339] = 100.07, p<0.001) and vaccination intentions (F[10–16,384] = 58.27, p<0.001) of participants in the 11 countries, the effects of the influenza label on worry (p = 0.281) and vaccination intentions (p = 0.467) did not significantly interact with country status.

Our results indicate that the choice of disease labels for public communications about outbreaks cannot be made by personal preference. In this study, an animal reservoir label evoked weaker responses from participants than other labels. Although these results could be specific to the animal we chose, using an animal reservoir label may produce greater misconceptions (e.g., exposure to the animal necessary for transmission) that undermine suspicions of risk. Further research is needed to determine whether this effect is context-specific or generalizes to other animal reservoir labels for infectious diseases and whether our findings replicate in a nonhypothetical context.

Dr. Scherer is an Associate of Internal Medicine at the University of Iowa. His research focuses on the psychological mechanisms that shape our responses to health risks in order to better design effective health risk communication.

Top

Acknowledgments

All authors contributed to the intellectual property of this manuscript, including contributing to study design, interpretation of data, and writing of the report. We have no competing interests to declare. All of the authors have had full access to the data, have seen and approved the submission of this version of the manuscript, and take full responsibility for the integrity of the data, the accuracy of the data analysis, and the manuscript.

Funding for this research was provided to A.F. from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme for research, technological development and demonstration under grant agreement #278763. The funding agreement assured the authors’ independence in designing the study, in the collection, analysis, and reporting of the data, and in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Top

References

  1. Scherer  AM, Scherer  LD, Fagerlin  A. Getting ahead of illness: using metaphors to influence medical decision making. Med Decis Making. 2015;35:3745. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Hauser  DJ, Schwarz  N. The war on prevention: bellicose cancer metaphors hurt (some) prevention intentions. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2015;41:6677. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Scherer  LD, Finan  C, Simancek  D, Finkelstein  JI, Tarini  BA. Effect of “pink eye” label on parents’ intent to use antibiotics and perceived contagiousness. Clin Pediatr (Phila). 2016;55:5438. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Scherer  LD, Zikmund-Fisher  BJ, Fagerlin  A, Tarini  BA. Influence of “GERD” label on parents’ decision to medicate infants. Pediatrics. 2013;131:83945. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Rothman  AJ, Bartels  RD, Wlaschin  J, Salovey  P. The strategic use of gain- and loss-framed messages to promote healthy behavior: how theory can inform practice. J Commun. 2006;56(s1):S20220. DOIGoogle Scholar
  6. Loewenstein  GF, Weber  EU, Hsee  CK, Welch  N. Risk as feelings. Psychol Bull. 2001;127:26786. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Determann  D, de Bekker-Grob  EW, French  J, Voeten  HA, Richardus  JH, Das  E, et al. Future pandemics and vaccination: Public opinion and attitudes across three European countries. Vaccine. 2016;34:8038. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar

Top

Figure

Top

Cite This Article

DOI: 10.3201/eid2308.170364

Table of Contents – Volume 23, Number 8—August 2017

EID Search Options
presentation_01 Advanced Article Search – Search articles by author and/or keyword.
presentation_01 Articles by Country Search – Search articles by the topic country.
presentation_01 Article Type Search – Search articles by article type and issue.

Top

Comments

Please use the form below to submit correspondence to the authors or contact them at the following address:

Aaron M. Scherer, Department of Internal Medicine, University of Iowa, 200 Hawkins Dr, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA

Send To

10000 character(s) remaining.

Top

Page created: July 17, 2017
Page updated: July 17, 2017
Page reviewed: July 17, 2017
The conclusions, findings, and opinions expressed by authors contributing to this journal do not necessarily reflect the official position of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, the Public Health Service, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, or the authors' affiliated institutions. Use of trade names is for identification only and does not imply endorsement by any of the groups named above.
file_external