Skip directly to site content Skip directly to page options Skip directly to A-Z link Skip directly to A-Z link Skip directly to A-Z link
Volume 28, Number 8—August 2022
Research

COVID-19 Vaccination Intent and Belief that Vaccination Will End the Pandemic

Author affiliations: National Institute for Public Health and the Environment, Bilthoven, the Netherlands (M. de Vries, L. Claassen, M. Lambooij, K.Y. Leung, A. Timen); Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (K. Boersma, A. Timen)

Cite This Article

Abstract

High vaccination coverage is considered to be key in dealing with the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) pandemic. However, vaccine hesitancy can limit uptake. We examined the specific coronavirus beliefs that persons have regarding COVID-19 and COVID-19 vaccines and to what extent these beliefs explain COVID-19 vaccination intentions. We conducted a survey among 4,033 residents of the Netherlands that examined COVID-19 vaccination intentions and various beliefs. Random forest regression analysis explained 76% of the variance in vaccination intentions. The strongest determinant in the model was the belief the COVID-19 crisis will only end if many persons get vaccinated. Other strong determinants were beliefs about safety of vaccines, specifically in relation to vaccine development and approval process; (social) benefits of vaccination; social norms regarding vaccination behavior; and effectiveness of vaccines. We propose to address these specific beliefs in communications about COVID-19 vaccinations to stimulate vaccine uptake.

The COVID-19 pandemic has profoundly affected global health and well-being. Since 2020, countries worldwide have experienced high rates of illness and death caused by COVID-19, and many societies have dealt with often stringent outbreak control measures. The successful development of effective vaccines has provided a much-wanted major step toward controlling the pandemic. However, for the vaccines to be successful during outbreak control, a high and equally distributed vaccine uptake is essential. Next to possible barriers of limited COVID-19 vaccine availability and accessibility, vaccine hesitancy can also form a considerable barrier to reaching a high vaccine uptake.

The public acceptance of vaccines has been a global concern for decades. Before the COVID-19 crisis, in 2019, the World Health Organization declared vaccine hesitancy as one of the top 10 global public health threats (1). Vaccine hesitancy has been defined as a broad range of vaccine-related attitudes and behavior, from having some doubts and delaying vaccinations up to complete refusal of vaccines (2). Various studies have provided insights into beliefs underlying vaccination hesitancy and vaccination intentions for childhood vaccinations (35); influenza vaccinations (5,6), including pandemic influenza A(H1N1) vaccination (57); and COVID-19 vaccinations (812). Personal beliefs that are known to have a major role in vaccination decision-making are beliefs about the need for, safety of, and effectiveness of vaccines.

Many studies that examine determinants of vaccination hesitancy, intentions, or behavior (e.g., studies applying the health belief model [913]) explore beliefs in relatively general terms. For example, surveys may simply ask respondents whether they have concerns about the safety of vaccines. It is useful to have more detailed knowledge of these beliefs for 3 reasons. First, in-depth insights into beliefs can provide more concrete input toward developing well-adapted communication (14). For example, concerns about safety of vaccines might be related to beliefs about the vaccine production process, long-term side-effects, and composition of vaccines. Such specific beliefs should be addressed in communications. Second, COVID-19 vaccination intentions are likely to be associated with specific beliefs that differ from those found in research during other vaccination campaigns. This reaction might be the case for beliefs about the rapid vaccine development process, the new technologies used (mRNA), and the personal freedom associated with vaccination (through vaccination entry passes). Third, there might be major differences in (the influence of certain) beliefs underlying vaccination decisions between countries or communities (15) (e.g., due to differences in experiences with the COVID-19 pandemic, information streams, and vaccination campaign history).

Consistent with earlier research on vaccination decisions (16), we adopted a mental models perspective in studying beliefs underlying COVID-19 vaccination intentions among persons in the Netherlands (14). This perspective entails a detailed study of interrelated beliefs of a subject, in this case COVID-19 and the COVID-19 vaccinations. These beliefs form a mental model underlying decisions of persons regarding COVID-19 vaccination. By gaining in-depth insights into these various beliefs, we can identify knowledge gaps and misbeliefs that need to be addressed in communications. In addition, by studying which beliefs are useful determinants of vaccination intentions, we aimed to identify beliefs that should be addressed and prioritized in communications to optimize vaccine acceptability and uptake.

Methods

Study Population and Procedure

We conducted an online survey during March 12–22, 2021. At that time, 1.5 million of the 17.5 million residents of the Netherlands were partly or fully vaccinated against COVID-19 (data from March 14, 2021) (17). We sent the survey (in Dutch) to 6,810 persons in the Netherlands (>18 years of age) by using an online survey panel (ISO/IEC 27001:2013; I&O Research, https://www.ioresearch.nl). Members from this survey panel were recruited by using random samples of name and address data. The sample invited for participation to this survey was selected to be representative of the general population of the Netherlands (>18 years of age) on the basis of demographic characteristics.

Panel members provided informed consent for participation to the survey panel. Survey completion took ≈10‒15 minutes. The Clinical Expertise Centre at the National Institute for Public Health and the Environment reviewed the study protocol and determined that the study was exempt from needing further approval from an ethics research committee (study no. LCI-485).

Development of Survey Measurements

We measured vaccination intention as follows. All respondents who indicated to have received an invitation for a COVID-19 vaccination but who had not yet been vaccinated were asked, “Do you want to get vaccinated against the corona virus?” Respondents who indicated not yet to be invited for a COVID-19 vaccination were asked, “If you are invited for a COVID-19 vaccination, do you then want to get vaccinated?” Both questions could be answered on a 5-point Likert scale: 1. Certainly not; 2. Probably not; 3. Don’t know; 4 Probably yes; 5. Certainly yes. The answers to both questions were taken together in 1 variable that was called COVID-19 vaccination intention.

To identify the beliefs about COVID-19 and the COVID-19 vaccines that should be assessed in this study, we studied literature on sociopsychological determinants of vaccination intentions to identify main elements in mental models of persons underlying COVID-19 vaccination intentions (2,4,18,19). In addition, we used recent qualitative studies (survey open answer categories and in-depth interviews) on COVID-19 vaccination acceptability among members of the public in the Netherlands (20,21) to identify specific beliefs within the familiar mental model themes and to identify beliefs that might not have been identified in studies on other vaccination campaigns.

We studied beliefs about COVID-19 and COVID-19 vaccines by using this question: “We would like to know what you think about the corona virus/vaccination against the corona virus. For each statement, indicate to what extent it aligns with what you think. I think ….” The question was followed by 25 statements that were scored on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = certainly not to 5 = certainly yes) (Table 1). The 25 beliefs can be categorized into 7 elements of mental models of persons, namely beliefs about risk for COVID-19 regarding oneself, risk for COVID-19 regarding one’s loved ones, safety of vaccination, effectiveness of vaccination, (social) benefits of vaccination, alternatives to vaccination, social norms regarding vaccination behavior, and accessibility of vaccination.

Analyses

We analyzed on responses of (at that time) unvaccinated respondents. Descriptive analyses were performed for vaccination intention and the 25 beliefs about COVID-19 and the vaccines. In addition, we calculated Pearson correlations (2-tailed) between vaccination intention and all 25 beliefs and between all beliefs separately.

We subsequently performed a regression analysis by using Random forest (RF) (22) in R (23) to assess the extent to which beliefs explain variance in vaccination intentions, and to identify which specific beliefs are good determinants of vaccination intentions (Appendix). RF is a machine learning method for regression and classification based on an ensemble of decision trees. This method makes no assumptions about data distribution and is well suited to address 3 complicating features of the study responses for analyses: a dependent variable (COVID-19 vaccination intention) that is not normally distributed, many (partly intercorrelated) independent variables (beliefs), and potentially nonlinear relationships between independent variables and the dependent variable. The RF method has been successfully applied in explaining vaccination intentions (16) and screening intentions (24). We controlled the RF regression analysis for age, sex, education level, region of residence, migration background, health status, previous coronavirus infection, being invited for a COVID-19 vaccination, perceived allergy for vaccinations, employment in healthcare, and religious motivations (Appendix) by adding these as independent variables to the RF model.

We considered 4 types of output from the RF regression analyses. The first output was the variable importance ranking (VIR), which ranks independent and control variables in terms of how much these contribute to explaining the dependent variable. The second output was the partial dependence (also known as marginal means) that indicates the direction and strength of the relationship between the independent and dependent variable. The third output was the cumulative variance explained, which is the variance explained after adding an independent variable to the model in the sequence of the VIR. The fourth output was the total variance explained.

Results

Study Population

The survey response rate was 59% (n = 4.033). The survey population was reasonably comparable to the general population in the Netherlands (>18 years of age) for demographic characteristics (Table 2).

COVID-19 Vaccination Intention

Most (2,266/3,628, 62.5%) unvaccinated respondents indicated that they would certainly want to get vaccinated against COVID-19, 645 (17.8%) would probably want to get vaccinated, 257 (7.1%) did not know (yet), 213 (5.9%) would probably not want to get vaccinated, and 247 (6.8%) indicated certainly not. The mean (+SD) response of vaccination intention was 4.2 (+1.2).

Beliefs about COVID-19 and COVID-19 Vaccines

Figure 1

COVID-19 vaccination intent and belief that COVID-19 vaccination will end the pandemic among persons in the Netherlands. Pearson correlation matrix (2-tailed) heat map with all beliefs about COVID-19 and COVID-19 vaccinations was visualized per mental models element (risk perceptions COVID-19: self, risk perceptions COVID-19: loved ones, safety vaccination, effectiveness vaccination, (social) benefits vaccination, alternatives to vaccination, social norms vaccination behavior, accessibility vaccination). For a more detailed correlation matrix, see Appendix.

Figure 1. COVID-19 vaccination intent and belief that COVID-19 vaccination will end the pandemic among persons in the Netherlands. Pearson correlation matrix (2-tailed) heat map with all beliefs about COVID-19 and COVID-19...

Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlations (2-tailed) with vaccination intention showed that all 25 beliefs were significantly (p<0.001) correlated with vaccination intention (Table 1). Correlations between different beliefs about COVID-19 and COVID-19 vaccinations (Figure 1) showed moderate-to-strong correlations between different risk perception beliefs of COVID-19 (for self and loved ones), and, at the same time, mostly weak correlations between these COVID-19 beliefs and the several beliefs about COVID-19 vaccinations. In addition, we observed many strong correlations between the various beliefs about COVID-19 vaccinations, especially in relation to beliefs about the safety of vaccination.

Personal risk perceptions of respondents for COVID-19 were moderate, with values of 2.9 (+1.0) for the belief about high likelihood of infection and 3.0 (+1.3) for the belief about possibility of severe illness. For loved ones, these COVID-19 risk perceptions were scored relatively higher: 3.5 (+1.1) for high likelihood of infection and 3.9 (+1.1) for possibility of severe illness. Respondents valued the likelihood to infect others if infected themselves with a value of 3.3 (+1.2).

Safety of vaccinations was generally trusted by respondents (Table 3), but some notable variations in responses were observed. For example, 27.7% of respondents indicated not believing that the side effects of vaccinated were well researched (mean 3.3, SD +1.3), and 28.3% of respondents believed that the vaccines were developed too quickly (mean 2.7, SD +1.3). With regard to the effectiveness of vaccinations, respondents believed that vaccines would protect them well against COVID-19 (mean 3.8, SD +1.1). Respondents seemed somewhat unsure about whether vaccines only protect for a short while (mean 3.1, SD +1.0) and whether one can still infect others after vaccination (mean 3.0, SD +1.1). At the time of data collection, scientific knowledge about those last 2 vaccine aspects was also limited.

In terms of (social) benefits of vaccinations, vaccinations were commonly seen as the only way out of the COVID-19 crisis (mean 4.1, SD +1.2) and as a means to return to a life without COVID-19 restrictions sooner (mean 3.8, SD +1.3). With regard to alternatives to vaccination, respondents generally did not believe that there were (sufficient) drugs that could cure COVID-19 (mean 1.9, SD +1.0), and few respondents believed that they were immune to COVID-19 (mean 2.0, SD +1.1). Some support was found for the belief that one’s good health would protect against COVID-19 (mean 2.8, SD +1.2).

We found that perceived social norms were generally in favor of vaccination. Beliefs that friends and family expected that the respondent would get vaccinated (mean 4.0, SD +1.2), and the beliefs that most of the respondent’s family and friends (mean 4.2, SD +1.0), as well as most residents in the Netherlands (mean 4.0, SD +0.8), would get vaccinated were largely supported. Accessibility of vaccination did not seem a large obstacle because the belief that getting vaccinated would take a lot of time or effort was not strongly supported among the respondents (mean 2.0, SD +1.2).

Variance in COVID-19 Vaccination Intention

Figure 2

COVID-19 vaccination intent and belief that COVID-19 vaccination will end the pandemic among persons in the Netherlands. Variable ranking random forest model shows the 10 strongest determinants. n = 3,614, explained variance 0.76, mean squared error 0.078 (dashed vertical line).

Figure 2. COVID-19 vaccination intent and belief that COVID-19 vaccination will end the pandemic among persons in the Netherlands. Variable ranking random forest model shows the 10 strongest determinants. n = 3,614,...

The random forest model explained 76% of the variance in COVID-19 vaccination intentions. This analysis was performed using data for 3,614 of the 3,628 unvaccinated respondents (14 respondents were excluded from the analysis because of missing values). We provide the VIR with the 10 best explaining beliefs (Figure 2). We also provide the cumulative variances explained and partial dependence of the 10 best explaining beliefs (Table 3). Of these 10 best explaining beliefs, 5 beliefs concern safety of vaccinations, 2 beliefs are about social benefits of vaccination, 2 beliefs concern social norms regarding vaccination behavior, and 1 belief is about effectiveness of vaccination.

There was no clear selection of the 25 beliefs that explained variance in vaccination intention considerably more strongly than all other beliefs. Instead we observe a gradual progression in explanatory value of various beliefs (VIR with all determinants; Appendix). Because there are many intercorrelations between the beliefs (Figure 1), and many of the beliefs are associated with vaccination intentions, the partial dependence ranges were also small (Table 3). Our findings confirm that vaccination decisions are made on the basis of a complex web of interrelated beliefs (mental models), rather than on a few independent perceptions. Although a small number of these beliefs can (statistically) explain a large part of the variance in vaccination intentions, one needs to keep in mind that in reality beliefs never stand on their own. This said, the belief “the corona crisis will only end if many people get vaccinated” seems, distinctively, the strongest determinant in the model. By adding only this variable to the model, we can explain 54% of the variance in vaccination intentions.

We conducted sensitivity analyses in which we repeated the main RF analyses for 3 age groups (18–34 years, 35–64 years, and >65 years). We repeated the main analysis with a binary dependent variable, explaining differences between those with low vaccination intentions (original values 1 and 2) and those who were unsure (value 3). Results were consistent with those of the main analyses and did not affect our conclusions.

Discussion

Our findings provide detailed insights into COVID-19 vaccination intentions and the underlying beliefs about COVID-19 and the COVID-19 vaccines among residents in the Netherlands during 2021. No major knowledge gaps or misbeliefs were observed, but we did observe some considerable concerns with regard to the vaccine development and approval process. The beliefs assessed in our study explained a large part of the variance in COVID-19 vaccination intentions. Beliefs about the safety of vaccines, (social) benefits of vaccination, social norms regarding vaccination behavior and the effectiveness of vaccines were, relative to other beliefs, strong determinants of vaccination intention for persons. The strongest determinant in the model was the belief “the corona crisis will only end if many people get vaccinated.”

Our study results showed strong beliefs, and the explanatory value of these beliefs, about (social) benefits after being vaccinated or reaching a high vaccination coverage. The belief that the COVID-19 crisis will only end if many persons get vaccinated could (statistically) explain more than half of the variance in COVID-19 vaccination intentions. It is striking that this belief seemed to be, at least somewhat, a better determinant of vaccination intentions than beliefs about personal protection against the vaccine-preventable disease or beliefs about safety of vaccines, which have often been identified as the most essential psychosocial determinants of vaccination intentions (5,17,18). The wish for relaxation of COVID-19 control measures and for the ending of the enduring crisis seem to have been stronger among many than the wish for personal protection against disease (although these wishes are not mutually exclusive). This finding might be explained by the considerable effect of COVID-19 measures on lives of persons (25) and the observed moderate COVID-19 risk perceptions. Our results also suggest that persons who did not believe that high vaccination coverage is the only solution to end the COVID-19 crisis were not less likely to vaccinate.

We might expect that over time fewer persons will have the belief of vaccination being the only solution to end the crisis, during the winter of 2021, when lockdown measures were again necessary, despite relatively high vaccination coverage (26). A decrease in this belief might lead to a decrease in vaccination acceptability. In communications, we are faced with a dilemma. In the short run, providing clear future perspectives regarding personal and societal benefits after reaching a high vaccination coverage, might considerably help in motivating persons to get vaccinated. At the same time, transparency about uncertainties regarding these perspectives are necessary from an ethical point of view, but also to prevent disappointments in the future resulting from too optimistic expectations. Transparency is also crucial in remaining trust and support for control measures (27,28).

Consistent with previous research, we found that various beliefs about the safety of the vaccines were major determinants of COVID-19 vaccination intentions (12,29). Five of the 10 major explanatory beliefs were related to safety. Four of these 5 beliefs were about vaccine development and approval processes. Rapid development of vaccines and the approval process, and the use of new techniques (e.g., mRNA vaccines) have probably increased public concerns about vaccine safety. Concerns about rapid development of vaccines were also observed in previous research on COVID-19 vaccination perceptions (8) and pandemic influenza A(H1N1) virus vaccination perceptions (30). Authorities must provide persons with timely and transparent information about development, approval, and safety monitoring of COVID-19 vaccines to fulfil their information needs. If such information is not, or is scarcely, provided by authorities, persons are likely to search for this information elsewhere on the internet, with the considerable risk that this would lead them to vaccine skeptical sources (31,32).

We showed that beliefs about descriptive and subjective social norms, specifically with regard to vaccination expectations and behavior of friends and family, were also major determinants of COVID-19 vaccination intentions. The role of social norms in vaccination behavior was also suggested in previous research with regard to COVID-19 vaccinations (8,33), influenza vaccinations (34,35), and human papillomavirus vaccinations (36,37). These findings suggest the potential for interventions focused on endorsing social norms with regard to vaccination (e.g., providing narratives in communication materials for peers who vaccinated). In addition, this finding might indicate that persons are, at least partly, segregated in like-minded groups on the basis of COVID-19 vaccination intentions, which could increase the risk for local outbreaks.

Beliefs about the health risks posed by COVID-19 were not found among the major determinants of vaccination intentions. A similar result was seen in a study on meningococcal vaccination intentions that had a similar study approach (16). An explanation for this result can lie in the distribution of responses in vaccination intentions. Because most of our respondents intended to vaccinate against COVID-19, the explanatory analysis shows mainly how persons who are not (so) willing to vaccinate differ from those who do want to vaccinate, because that is where the variance in responses can be found. Perceptions of the health risk posed by COVID-19 are likely major reasons for persons to vaccinate but might not be among the most essential reasons for those who do not intend to vaccinate.

The first limitation of our study is that, although the study population is at large fairly comparable with the population in the Netherlands in terms of main demographic characteristics, it is not a perfect representation. Presumably, there is an overlap between those persons who are difficult to reach for vaccination with those persons who are difficult to reach for research purposes. Second, our study was cross-sectional and conducted in a period that had rapid developments in information about COVID-19 vaccinations. For example, just before the start of our data collection, Denmark announced suspending use of vaccine from AstraZeneca (https://www.astrazeneca.com) after reports of possible severe adverse events; during the second half of our data collection, the Netherlands also temporarily suspended these vaccinations (38). Such developments might have affected the outcomes of our study (e.g., through a potential decrease in people’s trust in vaccine safety). Also, subsequent events might lead to slightly different results if the study was repeated. It would be highly valuable to repeat research like ours throughout prolonged crises and in multiple settings to monitor changes and differences. Third, our study focused on beliefs about COVID-19 and COVID-19 vaccinations to provide concrete input for communication. Our study did not address other possible major determinants of vaccination intentions, such as trust in institutions or health literacy. Fourth, we did not include beliefs about conspiracy theories in this study, which in hindsight could have added interesting insights. Such beliefs were not included because these were not pronounced in the literature nor in the qualitative data at the time we developed our survey.

Results of this study provide several essential key points for future research, policy, and communication. First, COVID-19 vaccinations decisions are not made purely by considering the pros and cons for one’s own health. Other (social) benefits of COVID-19 vaccination, related to the relaxation of COVID-19 control measures, are likely to play a major role in vaccination decisions of persons. Providing clear perspectives with regard to these benefits might increase vaccination uptake. At the same time, it is highly essential to address the uncertainties with regard to those social benefits and to prevent future disappointments and decreases in trust and support. Second, social norms regarding peers have been shown to be an essential factor in COVID-19 vaccination intentions, which suggests the potential for norms to induce interventions to increase vaccination uptake. Future research should focus on the characterization and identification of like-minded social networks who are hesitant to vaccinate against COVID-19 to provide well-tailored interventions. Finally, it is highly essential to provide transparent and accessible information about vaccine development and approval process and the probability of potential adverse events caused by vaccination to address concerns about safety of COVID-19 vaccines.

Dr. de Vries is a senior research scientist at the National Institute for Public Health and the Environment, Bilthoven, the Netherlands. Her primary research interests are social dynamics during disease outbreaks, risk and crisis communication, and vaccination behavior.

Top

Acknowledgments

We thank Kim van Zoonen, Christiaan Oostdijk, Renske Eilers, Anne-Romy Peelen, Floor Kroese, Koen van der Swaluw, Nora Hamdiui, Rob Ruiter, Vincent Buskens, Pepijn van Empelen, Danielle Timmermans, Mirjam Fransen, Ellen Uiters, and Marion Bouwer for contributing to survey development and colleagues from the panel organization I&O Research, specifically Jolijn Polet and Annemarijn Smit, for contributing to data collection.

This study was supported by the National Institute for Public Health and the Environment in the Netherlands and the European Union Horizon2020 project HERoS.

Top

References

  1. World Health Organization. Ten threats to global health in 2019 [cited 2022 Jun 15]. https://www.who.int/news-room/spotlight/ten-threats-to-global-health-in-2019
  2. Dubé  È, Ward  JK, Verger  P, MacDonald  NE. Vaccine hesitancy, acceptance, and anti-vaccination: trends and future prospects for public health. Annu Rev Public Health. 2021;42:17591. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Gidengil  C, Chen  C, Parker  AM, Nowak  S, Matthews  L. Beliefs around childhood vaccines in the United States: A systematic review. Vaccine. 2019;37:6793802. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Downs  JS, de Bruin  WB, Fischhoff  B. Parents’ vaccination comprehension and decisions. Vaccine. 2008;26:1595607. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Karafillakis  E, Larson  HJ; ADVANCE consortium. The benefit of the doubt or doubts over benefits? A systematic literature review of perceived risks of vaccines in European populations. Vaccine. 2017;35:484050. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Schmid  P, Rauber  D, Betsch  C, Lidolt  G, Denker  ML. Barriers of influenza vaccination intention and behavior—a systematic review of influenza vaccine hesitancy, 2005–2016. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0170550. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Prematunge  C, Corace  K, McCarthy  A, Nair  RC, Pugsley  R, Garber  G. Factors influencing pandemic influenza vaccination of healthcare workers—a systematic review. Vaccine. 2012;30:473343. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Latkin  CA, Dayton  L, Yi  G, Konstantopoulos  A, Boodram  B. Trust in a COVID-19 vaccine in the US: a social-ecological perspective. Soc Sci Med. 1982;2021:113684.
  9. Wong  MCS, Wong  ELY, Huang  J, Cheung  AWL, Law  K, Chong  MKC, et al. Acceptance of the COVID-19 vaccine based on the health belief model: A population-based survey in Hong Kong. Vaccine. 2021;39:114856. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Thaker  J. The persistence of vaccine hesitancy: COVID-19 vaccination intention in New Zealand. J Health Commun. 2021;26:10411. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Salmon  DA, Dudley  MZ, Brewer  J, Kan  L, Gerber  JE, Budigan  H, et al. COVID-19 vaccination attitudes, values and intentions among United States adults prior to emergency use authorization. Vaccine. 2021;39:2698711. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Karlsson  LC, Soveri  A, Lewandowsky  S, Karlsson  L, Karlsson  H, Nolvi  S, et al. Fearing the disease or the vaccine: The case of COVID-19. Pers Individ Dif. 2021;172:110590. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Zampetakis  LA, Melas  C. The health belief model predicts vaccination intentions against COVID-19: A survey experiment approach. Appl Psychol Health Well-Being. 2021;13:46984. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Morgan  MG. Risk communication: a mental models approach. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2002.
  15. Lazarus  JV, Ratzan  SC, Palayew  A, Gostin  LO, Larson  HJ, Rabin  K, et al. A global survey of potential acceptance of a COVID-19 vaccine. Nat Med. 2021;27:2258. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. de Vries  M, Claassen  L, Te Wierik  MJM, Coban  F, Wong  A, Timmermans  DRM, et al. Meningococcal W135 disease vaccination intent, the Netherlands, 2018‒2019. Emerg Infect Dis. 2020;26:14209. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Government of the Netherlands. Coronavirus dashboard: COVID-19 vaccinations, 2021 [cited 2021 Aug 11]. https://coronadashboard.rijksoverheid.nl/landelijk/vaccinaties
  18. Betsch  C, Böhm  R, Chapman  GB. Using behavioral insights to increase vaccination policy effectiveness. Policy Insights Behav Brain Sci. 2015;2:6173. DOIGoogle Scholar
  19. Larson  HJ, Cooper  LZ, Eskola  J, Katz  SL, Ratzan  S. Addressing the vaccine confidence gap. Lancet. 2011;378:52635. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. National Institute for Public Health and the Environment (RIVM). Corona behavioral unit in-depth interviews, round 9, 2021 [in Dutch] [cited 2021 Aug 11]. https://www.rivm.nl/documenten/corona-gedragsunit-verdiepende-interviews-ronde-9
  21. National Institute for Public Health and the Environment (RIVM). Participation in COVID-19 vaccination: update June 5, 2021 [in Dutch] [cited 2021 Aug 11]. https://www.rivm.nl/documenten/deelname-aan-covid-19-vaccinatie-stand-van-zaken
  22. Breiman  L. Random forests. Mach Learn. 2001;45:532. DOIGoogle Scholar
  23. Liaw  A, Wiener  M. Classification and regression by randomForest. R News. 2002;2:1822.
  24. Hamdiui  N, Stein  ML, Timen  A, Timmermans  D, Wong  A, van den Muijsenbergh  METC, et al. Hepatitis B in Moroccan-Dutch: a quantitative study into determinants of screening participation. BMC Med. 2018;16:47. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Serafini  G, Parmigiani  B, Amerio  A, Aguglia  A, Sher  L, Amore  M. The psychological impact of COVID-19 on the mental health in the general population. QJM. 2020;113:5317. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. National Institute for Public Health and the Environment (RIVM). Nearly 85% of people aged 18 and over have had at least one vaccination, 2021 [cited: 2021 Aug 11]. https://www.rivm.nl/en/news/nearly-85-of-people-aged-18-and-over-have-had-at-least-one-vaccination
  27. Seeger  MW. Best practices in crisis communication: an expert panel process. J Appl Commun Res. 2006;34:23244. DOIGoogle Scholar
  28. Covello  VT. Best practices in public health risk and crisis communication. J Health Commun. 2003;8(Suppl 1):58, discussion 148–51. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Sanders  JG, Spruijt  P, van Dijk  M, Elberse  J, Lambooij  MS, Kroese  FM, et al. Understanding a national increase in COVID-19 vaccination intention, the Netherlands, November 2020-March 2021. Euro Surveill. 2021;26:2100792. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Seale  H, Heywood  AE, McLaws  ML, Ward  KF, Lowbridge  CP, Van  D, et al. Why do I need it? I am not at risk! Public perceptions towards the pandemic (H1N1) 2009 vaccine. BMC Infect Dis. 2010;10:99. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Davies  P, Chapman  S, Leask  J. Antivaccination activists on the world wide web. Arch Dis Child. 2002;87:225. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Betsch  C, Renkewitz  F, Betsch  T, Ulshöfer  C. The influence of vaccine-critical websites on perceiving vaccination risks. J Health Psychol. 2010;15:44655. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Graupensperger  S, Abdallah  DA, Lee  CM. Social norms and vaccine uptake: College students’ COVID vaccination intentions, attitudes, and estimated peer norms and comparisons with influenza vaccine. Vaccine. 2021;39:20607. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Bruine de Bruin  W, Parker  AM, Galesic  M, Vardavas  R. Reports of social circles’ and own vaccination behavior: A national longitudinal survey. Health Psychol. 2019;38:97583. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Quinn  SC, Hilyard  KM, Jamison  AM, An  J, Hancock  GR, Musa  D, et al. The influence of social norms on flu vaccination among African American and White adults. Health Educ Res. 2017;32:47386. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. de Visser  R, Waites  L, Parikh  C, Lawrie  A. The importance of social norms for uptake of catch-up human papillomavirus vaccination in young women. Sex Health. 2011;8:3307. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Stout  ME, Christy  SM, Winger  JG, Vadaparampil  ST, Mosher  CE. Self-efficacy and HPV vaccine attitudes mediate the relationship between social norms and intentions to receive the HPV vaccine among college students. J Community Health. 2020;45:118795. DOIPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Government of the Netherlands. As a precaution, temporarily no vaccinations with AstraZeneca, 2021 [in Dutch] [cited 2021 Dec 12]. https://www.rijksoverheid.nl/actueel/nieuws/2021/03/14/uit-voorzorg-tijdelijk-geen-vaccinaties-met-astrazeneca

Top

Figures
Tables

Top

Cite This Article

DOI: 10.3201/eid2808.212556

Original Publication Date: July 07, 2022

Table of Contents – Volume 28, Number 8—August 2022

EID Search Options
presentation_01 Advanced Article Search – Search articles by author and/or keyword.
presentation_01 Articles by Country Search – Search articles by the topic country.
presentation_01 Article Type Search – Search articles by article type and issue.

Top

Comments

Please use the form below to submit correspondence to the authors or contact them at the following address:

Marion de Vries, National Institute of Public Health and the Environment, Centre for Infectious Disease Control, Antonie van Leeuwenhoeklaan 9, Bilthoven, Utrecht 3721 MA, the Netherlands

Send To

10000 character(s) remaining.

Top

Page created: June 15, 2022
Page updated: July 20, 2022
Page reviewed: July 20, 2022
The conclusions, findings, and opinions expressed by authors contributing to this journal do not necessarily reflect the official position of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, the Public Health Service, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, or the authors' affiliated institutions. Use of trade names is for identification only and does not imply endorsement by any of the groups named above.
file_external